Berkut. Vol. 11. Is. 2. 2002. P. 201-207.
Behaviour of
Montagu’s Harrier juveniles during
the post-fledging dependency period in southeast Poland
Ignacy Kitowski
Abstract. The dependency period of 51 fledglings
Montagu’s Harrier (Circus pygargus) was studied on calcareous
peat-bogs near Chelm (SE Poland). The juveniles fledged on average 33,6 days
after hatching, and continued to depend on their parents for 17–31 days (M =
23,6 ± 3,6 days, n = 42). A progression was observed in the flight
behaviour ability of the fledglings: the total time spend flying each day
increased throughout this period, as well as the use of energy saving flight
types. With progressing dependency period the rate of successfully aerial prey
transfers increased. The mortality rate during the dependency period was 17,6 %
(n = 51).
Key
words: Montagu’s Harrier, Circus pygargus,
Poland, behaviour, post-fledging dependency period, flight development.
Address:
I. Kitowski, Department of Nature Conservation, Maria
Curie-Sklodowska University Akademicka 19, 20-033 Lublin, Poland; e-mail: kitowign@biotop.umcs.lublin.pl.
Поведение молодых луговых
луней в послегнездовой период в Юго-Восточной Польше. - И. Китовский. - Беркут.
11 (2). 2002. - Послегнездовой период 51 слетка лугового луня изучался на известковых
верховых болотах возле г. Хелм (юго-восток Польши). Молодые птицы оставляли
гнездо в среднем через 33,6 дня после вылупления и докармливались родителями
еще на протяжении 17–31 дня (M = 23,6 ± 3,6 дней, n = 42). Отмечены
изменения в способности птенцов летать: с каждым днем увеличивается время,
проведенное в полете, и использование более экономных способов полета.
Увеличивается количество успешных передач корма в воздухе. Смертность во время
послегнездового докармливания составляла 17,6 % (n = 51).
INTRODUCTION
Despite the well known breeding ecology of
Montagu’s Harrier (Circus pygargus) (Schipper, 1978; Leroux,
Bretagnolle, 1996) the knowledge about the post-fledging period (hereafter
termed the dependency period) of this species is small. Studies on the
dependency period of Montagu’s Harrier have only been carried out in West
Europe, where they nest in agricultural areas (Pandolfi, 1996; Amar et al.,
2000). Little is known about the ecology of this species during the dependency
period in East-European peat-bogs. Furthermore, previous studies address only
some aspects of their ecology during that period, and the behaviour of the
juveniles is largely non-described, as well as the mortality of the juveniles
in such a critical period. The purpose of this study was to determine the
length of the dependency period for the species in SE Poland, estimate the
temporal trends in their behaviour (development of flight skills and hunting
behaviour, relationships with their parents and intruders), and determine the
rate of post-fledging mortality in the Montagu’s Harrier.
STUDY AREA AND METHODS
Observations were conducted in calcareous
peat-bogs near Chelm (51°08' N, 23°37' E, southeast Poland). Nearly 50 % of
this peatland is densely overgrown with Saw Sedge (Cladium mariscus)
and the remaining area, with associations of Sedge Magnocaricion
communities. This Saw Sedge habitat is a nesting site for about 30–50
pairs of Montagu’s Harrier depending on the season (Krogulec, 1992). The
climate of the study area is typical of southeast Poland, characterised by
relatively warm summers: mean temperature in July is 17,8 0C, in August 16,0 0C (Kaszewski et al., 1995).
In the years 1989–1992, 19 pairs of Montagu’s
Harriers with 51 fledglings were observed. Nestlings (n = 42) from 14 families
were individually Saflag® wing-tagged (Kochert et al., 1983). These patagial
tags appeared to have no negative effect on the fledglings’ behaviour during
our study. Some fledglings were found to lose their tags by the time for
migration. The other 9 untagged juveniles were distinguished by feather
characteristics (broken rectrices, primaries or wing coverts). Tagging
adult birds was given up as abandonment of broods after trapping was suspected
(Pandolfi, 1996). Adults were identified by their individual characteristics
including moult stage (Lontkowski, Stawarczyk, 1994). Moult proved particularly
useful in separating females. Differences in colour of wing coverts and feather
losses were useful details obtained from the onset of the incubation period.
Differences in head colour helped identify the males. In order to identify each
individual during the study, I recorded the differences in plumage by either
drawings or photographs of individuals. Age and sex of the offspring were
determined during nest examinations using plumage features and the colour of
iris (Krogulec,1992).
Field studies consisted of 12-hour observation
periods (800–2000), in a 1 to 4 day cycle for each of the
studied families. From 1989–1992, I conducted 152 observation sessions
totalling 1824 hours. The birds were observed with a 60x telescope and 10 x 60 binoculars at an average
distance of 200 m. Field studies started 2–3 days before the estimated first
flight (late June – early July). The duration of their behaviours was measured
with an electronic stopwatch and the height of juvenile’s flights was estimated
by comparing with height of trees of known height. Observations ended when the
last juvenile of each group left the natal area (late August). For the analyses
of the age, beginning and duration of the dependency period observations of all
51 fledglings were used. However, for analyses of behaviour only the
observations of the 37 colour marked juveniles were used. Because five wing
tagged birds dead.
Correlations used were Spearman rank
correlations or Pearson moment product correlations depending on the kind of
distribution of each variable (Sokal, Rohlf, 1981). To determine trends in the
behaviour of juveniles with their age, Pearson moment product correlation was
used. For variables for which the relationship with age of juveniles was
exponential rather than linear, I used Pearson moment product correlation after
log-transformation of the data. To compare age and duration of the dependency
period between the sexes, Student’s t- test was used. Frequencies were
compared using the c2 test with Yates corrections as necessary
(Sokal, Rohlf, 1981). All means are given ± SD.
Results
Age of first flights and duration of the dependency period
First flights were performed M = 33,6 ±
1,6 days after hatching (range 31–36 days after hatching, n = 51), and the
dependency period lasted on average of M = 23,6 ± 3,6 days (range 17–31
days, n = 42). Males performed their first flights at the age of M = 32,8
± 1,23 days (range 31–35 days, n = 23). Females started flying from M = 34,2
± 1,50 days after hatching (range 31–36 days, n = 28). The differences between
the sexes were significant (t = 3,576, P < 0,001, df = 49).
Young females remained under the parents’ supervision up to 52–66 days after
hatching (on average M =58,3 ± 3,63, n = 23). Young males remained with
adults on average up to M = 55,5 ± 3,48 days after hatching (range 52–65 days,
n = 19). Age differences between sexes at the beginning of dispersal were
significant (t = 2,557, P < 0,02, df = 40). Young males
dispersed 18–31 days after the first flight, on average M = 22,7 ± 3,31 days (n
= 19), whereas females dispersed 17–31 days since leaving the nest M = 24,3 ±
3,59 days (n = 23). The differences in the duration of the dependency period
between the sexes were insignificant (t = 1,380, n. s). Hatching date
(expressed in Julian date) did not influence the age of leaving the nest
(Spearman r = 0,123, n = 51, n. s). However, juveniles hatched earlier took
advantage of a longer care of adults (Spearman r = 0,591, n = 42, P < 0,002).
Flight development
The length of the first primary was measured on
day 30 after hatching in 7 fledglings performing the first flights on day 31
after hatching. The first primary was then only 72,3 % on average of that
of the first primary of adult Montagu’s Harriers reported by Cramp and Simmons
(1980). The first flights were only flapping flights. From M =40,1 ± 1,22 days
after hatching, (range 36-42, n = 11) the first short gliding flights were
observed. From M =49,0 ± 1,26 days after hatching (range 47–52, n = 16)
juveniles performed long lasting thermal soaring flights. The total daily time
spent flying (TDTSF) increased significantly with increasing juvenile age (r
= 0,750, n = 340, P < 0,0001), and the increase was exponential (y =
–26,56 x 8,7 )
The total daily time spent flapping by
juveniles did not change with their age (r = 0,162, n. s). However, the
percentage of time flying that was flapping as opposed to gliding decreased
with age (r = –0,752, n = 340, P < 0,0001), as the proportion of
flying time including soaring and gliding increased (r = 0,524, n = 340,
P< 0,0001). Due to that there appeared a strong correlation between TDTSF
and the duration of total daily time of soaring and gliding flights (r =
0,838, n = 340, P < 0,0001).
The maximal daily flight duration (MXFD) (r =
0,802, n = 340, P < 0,0001) and the mean daily flight duration (MEAFD) (r
= 0,803, n = 340, P < 0,0001) also increased exponential with age
of juveniles: y = –22,7 x7,3; y = –15,7 x5,2 respectively.
In the interval 31–48 days since hatching the
total daily number of flights (TDNF) increased with age (r = 0,710, n =
211, P < 0,0001). However, after that age no significant correlation was
noted between TDNF and the age of juveniles (r = –0,238, n = 129, n.
s). In the first period after fledging, TDNF was significantly and positively
correlated with all other variables describing flight duration: MAXFD, MEAFD
and TDTSF. After 48 days of age, only TDTSF was significantly correlated with
TDNF (Table 1).
Table 1
Pearson’s correlation coefficients (r)
and regression coefficients (a) between variables describing flight
ability of Montagu’s Harrier juveniles and the total daily number of flights
(TDNF)
Коэффициенты корреляции (r) и регрессии (a)
между переменными, описывающими способность к полету молодых луговых луней и
общее количество полетов в день
Variables Age of juveniles (days after hatching)
31–48,
n = 211 49–66, n = 129
Total
daily time spent flying (TDTSF) r =
0,940**, a = 1,69 r =
0,721**, a = 0,963
Mean daily
flight duration (MEAFD) r =
0,768**, a = 0,721 r =
–0,255, a = –2,377
Maximal
daily flight duration (MXFD) r =
0,845**, a = 1,144 r =
0,238, a = 0,367
** P < 0,0001
Nest ties
The total daily time spent by juveniles in the
nest (TDTSN) decreased with age (r = –0,749, n = 340, P < 0,0001), as
did the number of flights starting or finishing in the nest (r = –0,441,
n = 340, P < 0,0001). A strong negative correlation between TDNF and TDTSN
occurred for 31–48 days since hatching (r = 0,743, n = 211, P <
0,0001). Such a relationship was not observed in the second half of the
dependence period (r = –0,190, n = 129, n. s). For the whole dependency
period a negative correlation was shown between TDTSN and either MEAFD (r =
–0, 448, n = 340, P < 0,0001) or MAXFD (r = –0,401, n = 340, P <
0,0001).
Aerial food transfers
Initial prey transfers from adult to juveniles
were in the nest and on the ground (Table 2). On average, fledglings
successfully caught prey from parents in the air from M= 41,5 ± 8,86
days after hatching (range 36–46 days, n = 23). The percentage of successfully
caught prey increased through the dependency period (r = 0,747, n = 340,
P < 0,0001). Of n = 774 aerial food transfers between parents and
offspring, 85,3 % were successful. The success of aerial transfers was
negatively correlated with TDTSN (r = –0,682, n = 340, P < 0,0001)
and positively correlated with the maximal daily height of their flights (r =
0,512, n = 340, P < 0,0001).
Table 2
Types of food transfers from parents to
dependant offspring according to offspring age (days after fledging)
Типы передачи корма от родителей опекаемым птенцам
соответственно их возрасту (дни после вылета)
Type
of food pass 1–11 days 12–22
days 23–31 days
On the
nest transfers 579 177 –
On the
ground transfers 62 42 14
Aerial 28 324 422
Total 669 543 436
Parent-offspring relationships
The mean time spent on begging flights (MTBF),
where juveniles follow their parents emitting soliciting calls, increased with
progressing dependence period (r = 0,679, n = 340, P < 0,0001).
However, the total number of daily flights towards parents did not change
significantly with the age of juveniles (r = 0,176, n. s.). During
feeding, 12 cases of aggression of juveniles towards parents were observed.
From M = 49,5 ± 3,2 days after hatching, (range 47–52 days, n = 10)
unsuccessful cases of juvenile harriers begging for food from strange birds
were noted (Table 3).
Table 3
Frequency of occurrence of begging behaviour by
juveniles of Montagus Harrier directed to non-parents
Частота выпрашивания корма молодыми луговыми лунями,
обращенного к не родителям
Species Number
cases
Circus
pygargus 47
C.
aeruginosus 24
Falco
tinunnculus 2
Aquila
pomarina 3
Ciconia
nigra 4
C.
ciconia 6
Larus
ridibundus 3
Sterna
hirundo 3
Ardea
cinerea 5
Total 97
Harassing of intruders
Juveniles started to chase aggressively
intruders from M = 44,5 ± 3,2 days after hatching (range 37–48, n = 27).
As juveniles matured, the frequency of aggressive chases increased (r =
0,464, n = 340, P < 0,0001). However, that was not accompanied with
an increase of the total daily time spent on harassing intruders (TDTSH) (r =
0,259, n. s). TDTSH was significantly correlated positively with TDTSF (r =
0,447, n = 340, P < 0,0001) and TDNF (r = 0,554, n = 340, P < 0,0001).
A significant relationship also occurred between MTBF and the number of
aggressive chases of intruders (r = 0, 516, n = 340, P < 0,0001).
Hunting behaviour
As juveniles grew up a significant increase in
the number of cruising flights (sensu Jimenez, Jaksic, 1989) was
observed (r = 0,503, n = 340, P < 0,001), which are typical of the
hunting behaviour of Montagu’s Harrier. From M = 48,5 ± 2,9 after
hatching (range 42–55 days, n = 7) juveniles tried to catch
dragonflies (Odonata) in the air. However, no attempt was successful.
Successful hunting of grasshoppers (Tettigonioidea) was recorded from M
= 49,0 ± 5,54 days after hatching (range 47–54 days).
Unsuccessful attempts to catch passerines were observed on seven occasions M =
47,3 ± 3,23 days after hatching (range 45–52 days).
Post-fledging mortality
Nine (17,6 %) of the 51 studied juveniles were
found dead. The death of 7 of those juveniles (77,7 %) was caused by foxes (Vulpes
vulpes). One juvenile was victim of cainism (Kitowski, 1994b) and another
one was killed by an undetermined raptor. It could have been killed by Goshawks
(Accipiter gentilis), Lesser Spotted Eagle (Aquila pomarina),
Marsh Harrier (Circus aeruginosus). During the study, no death of
juvenile because of starvation was detected.
DISCUSSION
The duration of the dependency period in SE
Poland appeared to be similar to that found in Italy (on average 24 days,
Pandolfi, 1996) and in western France (Amar et al., 2000), where 35 wild
fledglings remained on average 25 days. However, dependency period in peatland
areas of SE Poland was shorter than that observed by Pomarol (1994) in Spain,
where the duration of the dependence period of hacked juveniles was 33,7 days.
The results from this study confirm those
obtained previously from a smaller sample in relation to the age of first
flights (Kitowski, 1994b): males of Montagu’s Harrier left the nest at a
younger age than females, as observed in other raptors with reversed sexual
dimorphism (Schaarf, Balfour, 1971; Newton 1986; Witkowski, 1989). This
suggests that male Montagu’s Harriers develop more quickly than their female
siblings. As in other raptors (Donazar, Ceballos, 1989; Schaadt, Bird,
1993) young Montagu’s Harriers started flights when their primaries were not
fully grown. It also appeared that in young Ospreys (Pandion haliaetus)
feathers were the most weakly developed body element of offspring at the time
of their first flights (Schaadt, Bird, 1993).
The development of flight skills in juvenile
Montagu’s Harriers seemed to take place like in other raptors (Bustamante,
Hiraldo, 1989; Bustamante, 1993), which increase the total, mean and maximal
daily time spent on flights throughout the dependency period. In the Montagu’s
Harrier, these variables increased exponentially rather than linearly with age.
Therefore, by the end of the dependency period the increase in flying time was
accelerated. This was accompanied with a decrease in the number of flights (as
each flight bout lasted longer). The disruption of the strong relation
relationship between time spent flying and number of flights typical of the
first part of dependency period opens unequivocally “the window of dispersion”
(Kenward et al., 1993). The time of the departure from the natal area coincided
with the peak of flight skills of juveniles. The disappearance of anatomical
constraints due to full development of feathers and their hardening (Brown,
Amadon, 1968; Bustamante, Hiraldo, 1989) was associated with an
extension of the range of flight techniques: gliding and soaring were used by
juveniles more frequently in the second part of the dependency period. These
techniques require less energy in comparison with the flapping flights
(Pennycuick, 1989) typical of the early dependency period.
Soaring and gliding, in contrast to other
raptors (Brown, 1990; Bustamante, 1993) are insignificant for foraging of
Harriers which used frequently crusing (sensu: Jimenez, Jaksic,
1989) what is result of application of hearing for prey detection (Schipper,
1977; Rice, 1982). However, the ability to perform such flight techniques may
also be important for juveniles in the context of the autumn migration (Spaar,
1996).
Offspring of Montagu’s Harrier were very
efficient during aerial food transfers, similarly than in Central Italy – 89 %
successful aerial food transfers (n = 131, Pandolfi, 1996) and even more so
than African Marsh Harrier (Circus ranivorus) – 78 % (n = 73, Simmons,
1991). The success of aerial food transfers in the studied juveniles depended
on the development of skills in flying. Better flight skills allowed them to
shorten the distance to the talons of parent, which assured them to grasp the
prey.
The
results of this study also show that breaking links with the nest by juveniles
of Montagu’s Harrier resulted from performing a larger number of flights rather
than increasing their duration. The ability to make frequent short flights
despite not having completely developed feathers may play an adaptive role in
this ground-nesting species, reducing the risk of predation by mammals in the
early dependency period. The latter is particularly important as the results
from this study also show that death by mammal predation is an important risk
for juvenile Montagu’s Harriers during that period.
The mortality rate of Montagu’s Harriers during
the post-fledging period observed in this study is relatively high as compared
to that observed in Accipitridae raptor species nesting in trees, such as Black
Kite (Milvus migrans) (6,7 %, n = 15, Bustamante, Hiraldo, 1989), Ferruginous
Hawk (Buteo regalis) (11,1 %, n = 18, Konrad, Gilmer, 1986),
or Red Kites (Milvus milvus) (13,5 %, n =37, Bustamante, 1993), but
closer to that observed in ground-nesting Hen Harriers (Circus cyaneus)
(14,2 %, n = 7, Beske, 1982). In falcons, however, a higher post-fledging
mortality rate was recorded than in Montagu’s Harriers. Of 61 radiotagged
juveniles of American Kestrels (Falco sparverius), 26 % died (Varland,
1993). Among young Mexican Falcons (F. mexicanus) 31 % (n = 152) died
before the dispersion (McFadzen, Marzluff, 1996). Of 25 juveniles of Lesser
Kestrels (F. naumanni) 51% died (Bustamante, Negro, 1994).
In all species, the two main causes of
mortality after leaving the nest are predation (Varland et al., 1993)
and starvation (Bustamante, Negro 1994). Other causes such as collisions with
trees, premature flights because of windy conditions, etc, occur occasionally,
but they do not seem to have a big influence (Brown, 1990). In the Montagu’s
Harrier, attacks of mammalian predators (Carnivora) were the main reason
of death, similar than what was observed in American Kestrels (Varland et al.,
1993) and Black Kites (Bustamante, Hiraldo, 1989). However, attacks by raptors
also occurred. Attacks of diurnal raptors such as Goshawks caused considerable
losses among emancipation young Sparrow Hawks
(Accipiter nisus) (Newton, 1986), and may also be important for
the Montagu’s Harriers.
Acknowledgments. I
am greatly indebted to: Dr. Beatriz Arroyo, (Centre for Ecology an Hydrology,
Banchory Hill of Brathens, Scotland, UK), and Dr. Patricia Kennedy
(Dept. Fishery & Wildl. Biology, Colorado State Univ., Ft. Collins, USA)
for helpful comments.
REFERENCES
Amar A., Arroyo B.E., Bretagnole V. (2000):
Post-fledging dependence and dispersal in hacked and wild Montagu’s Harrier
Circus pygargus. - Ibis. 142: 21-28.
Beske A. E. (1982): Local and migratory
movements of radiotagged juvenile harriers. - Raptor Res. 16: 39-53.
Bustamante J. (1993): Post-fledging period and
development of flight and hunting behaviour in Red Kite Milvus milvus.
- Bird Study. 40: 181-188.
Bustamante J., Hiraldo F. (1989): Post-fledging dependence and
maturation of flight skills in the Black Kite Milvus migrans. - Bird
Study. 36: 199-204.
Bustamante J., Negro J.J. (1994): The post-fledging dependence
period of the Lesser Kestrel (Falco naumanni) in Southwestern Spain. -
J. Raptor Res. 28: 158 -163.
Brown L., Amadon D. (1968): Eagles, Hawks and
Falcons of the World. New York: McGraw-Hill. 1: 1-431.
Brown C. J. (1990): Breeding Biology of the
Bearded Vulture in Southern Africa. Part III: The post-nestling dependency
period. - Ostrich. 61: 43- 49.
Cramp S., Simmons K.E.L. (Eds.). (1980): The birds
of Western Palearctic. Oxford: Oxford. Univ. Press. 2: 1-695.
Donazar J.A., Ceballos O. (1989): Growth rates
of nesting Egyptian Vultures (Neophron percnopterus) in relation to
brood size, hatching order and environmental factors. - Ardea. 77: 217-226.
Ferrer M. (1992): Regulation of postfledging
dependence in Spanish Imperial Eagle Aquila adalberti. - Ibis. 134:
128-133.
Jimenez J. E., Jaksic F. (1989): Behavioural
ecology of Grey Eagle-Buzzards, Geranoaetus melanoleucus, in central
Chile. - Condor. 91: 913-921.
Kaszewski B.M., Brugala S., Warakomski W.
(1995): [Environment of Lubelszczyzna Region. Climate]. Lublin: LTN. 1-126. (in Polish).
Kenward R.E., Marcstrom V., Karlbom M. (1993):
Post-nestling behaviour in goshawks Accipiter gentilis: I. causes of
dispersal. - Anim. Behav. 46: 365-370.
Kitowski I.(1994a): [Post-fledging period
ecology of Montagu’s Harrier Circus pygargus on calcareous marshes near
Chelm]. - PhD Thesis. Lublin: Univ. of Maria Curie-Skłodowska. 1-128. (in
Polish).
Kitowski I. (1994b): Montagu’s Harrier Circus
pygargus post-fledging period activities in eastern Poland – preliminary
results. – Raptor conservation today. Berlin: The Pica Press. 147-150.
Kitowski I. (1998): [Group mobbing on birds and
foxes by Montagu’s Harrier Circus pygargus in the period of fledglings
emancipation]. - Not. Ornitol. 39: 211-217. (in Polish).
Kochert M.N., Steenhof K., Moritsch M.Q. (1983):
Evaluation of patagial markers for raptors and ravens. - Wildl. Soc. Bull. 11:
271-281.
Konrad P.M., Gilmer D.S. (1986): Post-fledging
behaviour of Ferruginous Hawks in North Dakota. - Raptor Res. 20: 35-39.
Krogulec J. (1992): [Breeding Ecology of
Montagu’s Harrier Circus pygargus near Chelm]. - PhD Thesis. Lublin:
Univ. of Maria Curie-Skłodowska. 1-124. (in Polish).
Leroux A., Bretagnolle, V. (1996): Sex ratio
variations in broods of Montagu’s Harriers Circus pygargus. - J. Avian
Biol. 27: 63-69.
Lontkowski J., Skakuj M. (1994): [Field
identification of the Hen, Montagu’s and Pallid Harriers]. - Not. Ornitol. 35:
347-371. (in Polish).
McFadzen M.E., Marzluff J. M. (1996): Mortality
of prairie falcons during the fledging-dependence period. - Condor. 98:
791-800.
Newton I. (1986): The Sparrowhawk. Calton:
Poyser. 1-400.
Pandolfi M. (1996): Play activity in young
Montagu’s Harrier (Circus pygargus). - Auk. 113: 935-938.
Pennycuick C. J. (1989): Bird Flight
Performance. A Practical Calculation. New York: Oxford Univ. Press. 1-239.
Pomarol M (1994): Releasing Montagu’s Harrier (Circus
pygargus) by method of hacking. - J. Raptor Res. 28: 19-22 .
Rice W. (1982): Acoustical location of prey by
the Marsh Hawk. Adaptation to concealed prey. - Auk. 99: 403-413.
Schaadt C.P., Bird D. M. (1993): Sex-specific
growth in osprey: role of sexual size dimorphism. - Auk. 110 : 900-910.
Schaarf W.C., Balfour, E. (1971): Growth and
development of nestling Hen Harriers. - Ibis. 113: 323-329.
Schipper W. J. A. (1977): Hunting in three
European Harriers (Circus) during the breeding season. - Ardea. 65:
53-72.
Schipper W. J. A. (1978): A comparison of
breeding ecology in three European Harriers (Circus). - Ardea. 66:
77-102.
Simmons R. (1988): Honest advertising, sexual
selection, courtship displays and body condition of polygynous male harriers. -
Auk. 105: 303-307.
Simmons R. (1991): The efficiency and evolution
of aerial food passing in Harriers. - Gabar. 6: 51-56
Sokal R.R , Rohlf F. J.(1981): Biometry. San
Francisco: Freeman. 1-334.
Spaar R. (1996): Flight behaviour of migrating
raptors in southern Israel . - PhD Thesis. University of Basel. Sempach: Verlag Schweizerische
Vogelwarte. 1-148.
Varland D.E., Klass E.E, Loughin T.M. (1993 ):
Use habitat and perches, causes of mortality and time until dispersal in
post-fledging American Kestrels. - J. Field Ornithol. 67: 680-690.
Witkowski J. (1989): Breeding biology and
ecology of the Marsh Harrier Circus aeruginosus in the Barycz valley,
Poland. - Acta Orn. 25: 223-320.